Leukocytes navigate through complex chemoattractant arrays, and in so doing, they must migrate from one chemoattractant source to another. By evaluating directional persistence and chemotaxis during neutrophil migration under agarose, we show that cells migrating away from a local chemoattractant, against a gradient, display true chemotaxis to distant agonists, often behaving as if the local gradient were without effect. We describe two interrelated properties of migrating cells that allow this to occur. First, migrating leukocytes can integrate competing chemoattractant signals, responding as if to the vector sum of the orienting signals present. Second, migrating cells display memory of their recent environment: cells' perception of the relative strength of orienting signals is influenced by their history, so that cells prioritize newly arising or newly encountered attractants. We propose that this cellular memory, by promoting sequential chemotaxis to one attractant after another, is in fact responsible for the integration of competitive orienting signals over time, and allows combinations of chemoattractants to guide leukocytes in a step-by-step fashion to their destinations within tissues.

Exquisitely choreographed cell movements underlie many fundamental biological processes. In embryological development, cells migrate together to form complex organs, and neurons follow tortuous courses to their destinations. Similarly, immunologic surveillance requires coordinated, highly specific cell movements; to combat invading pathogens, neutrophils and monocytes must migrate to specific sites within infected tissues, and to respond to foreign antigens, lymphocytes must circulate through the body visiting specific functional zones within lymphoid organs. The mechanisms that direct and control cell movements with such specificity are just beginning to be elucidated.

A diverse group of chemoattractant molecules are thought to play a critical role in directing leukocyte homing within tissues. For many years, several classical chemoattractants produced in inflamed tissues, such as bacterial peptides, have been known to elicit neutrophil migration. In recent years, a multitude of related protein chemoattractants for leukocytes, called chemokines, have been characterized (for reviews see Baggiolini et al. 1997; Luster 1998; Zlotnik et al. 1999). Both chemokines and classical attractants bind to G protein–coupled receptors on leukocyte surfaces, and elicit cell orientation and directional migration when presented in a concentration gradient. Mice deficient in the ability to produce or respond to chemokines or classical attractants can display marked defects in leukocyte localization to and within their recruiting tissues (Cook et al. 1995; Förster et al. 1996; Gao et al. 1997; Hopken et al. 1997; Kurihara et al. 1997).

The characterization of a wide variety of leukocyte attractants and their receptors has painted a complex picture of the migratory environment within leukocyte-recruiting tissues. First, studies have shown that each leukocyte type can respond to multiple different chemoattractants (Baggiolini et al. 1997; Luster 1998; Zlotnik et al. 1999). Furthermore, in general, a diverse array of chemoattractants is produced by different cell types within leukocyte-recruiting tissues (Becker et al. 1994; Schroder 1995; Wenzel and Abboud 1995; Gillitzer et al. 1996; Glabinski et al. 1996; Gonzalo et al. 1996; Spanaus et al. 1997; Luster 1998). Therefore, a leukocyte entering a recruiting tissue likely encounters many different chemotactic signals to which it can respond. In a recent study, we showed that in such settings, neutrophils can navigate to their target by migrating in sequence toward one chemoattractant source and then another (Foxman et al. 1997). This behavior permits combinations of chemoattractants to act in series to guide cells to specific destinations. To find their way through such a complex chemoattractant environment, cells require mechanisms to integrate and prioritize the signals they receive.

Some mechanisms whereby leukocytes prioritize chemotactic signals are known. For example, neutrophils can prioritize signals from their phagocytic targets over more general recruitment signals (Foxman et al. 1997), likely via heterologous receptor desensitization by dominant agonists (Wilde et al. 1989; Dobos et al. 1992; Tomhave et al. 1994; Richardson et al. 1995; Campbell et al. 1997; Foxman et al. 1997; Kitayama et al. 1997; Sabroe et al. 1997). This ability is expected to allow neutrophils to find their end targets efficiently, even in settings in which regulatory chemoattractant gradients are also present (Campbell, 1997; Foxman et al. 1997). However, of the many leukocyte chemoattractants that have been described, dominant agonists appear to be exceedingly outnumbered by chemoattractants, such as the chemokines, that do not suppress cellular responses to other attractants. When a leukocyte encounters overlapping arrays of these nondominant, generally regulatory cell–derived agonists, the chemoattractant receptors involved do not engage in cross-desensitization, but rather signal simultaneously within the cell. However, even in the presence of such nondominant agonists, cells must be able to prioritize chemotactic signals so that they can successfully navigate through chemoattractant arrays. In particular, to respond in series to sequentially encountered gradients, leukocytes must be able to migrate away from one chemoattractant source towards another, as indeed they can (Foxman et al. 1997; Kitayama, 1997).

To learn more about how leukocytes process conflicting chemotactic signals, we studied neutrophil orientation in the under agarose assay (Foxman et al. 1997) evaluating the effects of directional persistence versus chemotaxis on cell behavior in the presence of opposing gradients of two attractants: the chemokine interleukin 8 (IL-8)1 and the classical lipid attractant leukotriene B4 (LTB4). These agonists are both produced by regulatory cells in inflamed tissues, and neither suppresses cellular responses to the other (Foxman et al. 1997). We demonstrate that cells migrating away from a local chemoattractant source actually chemotax towards distant attractants. However, neutrophils can clearly experience and respond as if to the vector sum of orienting signals from competing chemoattractants, resulting in migration directed between the two agonist sources (when the two independent attractants are presented equidistant at an angle). Reconciling these observations, we demonstrate that a cell's chemotactic bias in a given chemoattractant array depends not only on the gradients present, but also on the cell's memory of its recent chemoattractant environment. We propose a model in which a delay in adaptation constitutes this cellular memory and, by allowing cells to prioritize newly arising or distant attractants, leads to the integration of conflicting chemotactic signals over time and the step-by-step navigation of leukocytes in response to sequentially encountered chemoattractant sources.

Under Agarose Assay

Assay Procedures.

The procedures of Nelson et al. 1975 were adapted for our experiments, as described previously (Foxman et al. 1997), with modifications as outlined in Results. Neutrophils were isolated using standard techniques (see Foxman et al. 1997). For each cell tracking experiment, assays were set up and allowed to incubate at 37°C for 75 min, at which time they were moved to a warmed (37°C) microscope stage, and cell movements were recorded for 15 min.

Agarose Gel.

Agarose gels contained endotoxin-tested RPMI-1640 (Sigma Chemical Co.), 10% heat-inactivated bovine calf serum (serum inactivated at 57°C for 25 min), 1.2% agarose (GIBCO BRL), and 10 mM Hepes (GIBCO BRL), pH 7.2. For standard assays, five 3.5-mm-diam holes were cut 2- or 1.5-mm apart in a linear array. For experiments with three equidistant wells in an equilateral triangle, a template-guided steel punch was used to form three 3-mm-diam wells 3.3-mm apart.

Migration Medium.

Cells and agonists were suspended in RPMI-1640, 10% heat-inactivated bovine calf serum, and 10 mM Hepes, pH 7.2.

Chemoattractants.

Chemoattractants used were Leukotriene B4 (Sigma Chemical Co.) and recombinant human interleukin-8 (a gift from Antal Rot, Sandoz-Forschungsinstitut, Vienna, Austria).

Video and Confocal Microscopy.

Images were captured every 30 s on an inverted microscope (captured area = 780 μm orthogonal to the axis of the wells × 584 μm along axis of wells) or every 15 s on a laser scanning confocal microscope (MRC 1024; Bio-Rad Laboratories, Inc.) using Lasersharp software version 2.1 (Bio-Rad; captured area = 800 × 800 μm). For confocal microscopy experiments, neutrophils were labeled with 100 nM (final concentration/107 cells) of either SYTO-13 (green) or SYTO-15 (orange; Molecular Probes Inc.) for 25 min at room temperature in migration medium, washed, resuspended, and stored on ice until the start of the assay.

Uniform Field of LTB4.

To create a uniform field of chemoattractant, 1 nM LTB4 was incorporated into the agarose gel. Two wells, 7.5 mm apart, were cut in the gel. At t = 0, each well was filled with 105 cells in 20 μl of medium containing 1 nM LTB4. At t = 60 min (15 min before filming), well contents were overlaid with agarose containing 1 nM LTB4.

Data Analysis

Cell Tracking.

Each videotape (or series of confocal images) was converted to an NIH Image time-lapse movie, with one image every 30 (or 15) s. Cells moved on average 1–1.5 cell lengths/min, so cells moved less than a cell length between frames and could be identified from one image to the next. (The length of migrating cells was ∼20 μm as cells spread out significantly under the agarose.) An analysis region was defined based on the position of the filmed cells relative to the chemoattractant-containing wells, and only cells that were the desired distance from each well at the beginning of the movie were tracked. The analysis region always excluded cells within 100 μm of the field edge because they could not be reliably tracked for the entire 11.5-min analysis period. Usually, 90% of the cells within the analysis region could be followed for the entire 11.5-min analysis period (always = 87.5%). Once the analysis region was defined, each cell was numbered and its x, y coordinates were measured on the first image and on every subsequent image in the image stack, with the x-axis parallel to the edge of the cell starting well and the y-axis orthogonal to the well edge (parallel to a line connecting the starting well and the distant well). The (x, y) data for each cell was exported to a Microsoft EXCEL spreadsheet and used for the subsequent calculations of chemotactic bias.

The filmed field was aligned so that the axis of the experimental wells on the plate passed vertically through the center of the field, with the forward direction defined as directly away from the cell starting well.

Determination of the Decay of Cellular Persistence.

For cells migrating in a uniform field, the direction of a cell's motion during a given time interval was calculated trigonometrically using the cell's (x, y) coordinates. To determine an average direction at each time point, the cell's angle relative to the forward direction (0°) was calculated over three overlapping time intervals; for example, the cell's initial direction was calculated as the average of the cell's angle from 0 to 1 min, 0.5 to 1.5 min, and 1 to 2 min of observation. A cell was considered to be persistent until it achieved an angle >90° from its starting angle. To assure accurate assessment of the starting angle, the few cells that were not traveling at least 1 cell length/min (20 μm/min) during the initial 2-min period were excluded from the analysis. The analysis was begun within 3 min of the start of the videotape, at the first time at which the cell was traveling at least 20 μm/min.

Determination of Chemotactic Bias.

Each cell's (x, y) coordinates were used to calculate the cell's efficiency of forward migration, or forward migration index (FMI), during an initial 2-min analysis period, and its efficiency of forward migration during a subsequent test period of 4–9.5 min (see Results). The FMI was calculated as follows:

\begin{equation*}{\mathrm{FMI}}=\frac{{\mathrm{Forward\;Progress}}}{{\mathrm{Total\;Path\;Length}}}=\frac{{\mathrm{{\Sigma}{\Delta}y}}}{{\mathrm{{\Sigma}}} \left \left( \left \left({\mathrm{{\Delta}x}}^{2}+{\mathrm{{\Delta}}}y^{2}\right) \right ^{-{1}/{2}}\right) \right }\end{equation*}
1

with Δx and Δy assessed for each 30-s interval throughout the analysis period. For most experiments, the FMI was only calculated for the initial 2 min of observation and the subsequent 9.5 min; 11.5 min was the longest period for which nearly all cells selected could be followed without interruption (without leaving the field), and thus, an initial 2-min and subsequent 9.5-min observation provides the most reliable measure of the cells' time-averaged behavior. For each experimental condition, we plotted the initial versus subsequent FMI for all tracked cells and fitted a line through these data points by linear regression. The y-intercept of this line indicates the cells' chemotactic bias (see Results).

Statistical Methods.

We used standard statistical techniques to weigh and pool data acquired on different days. Intercepts of regression lines were compared using a t test.

Effect of Directional Persistence on Neutrophil Migration

To understand the behavior of neutrophils migrating in overlapping chemoattractant gradients, we needed to develop a quantitative method to evaluate chemotaxis (directed cell motility) and to distinguish its contribution from the other factors influencing cell migration in our model. We first characterized cell migration in the presence of a uniform field of chemoattractant: chemoattractants stimulate random cell motility (chemokinesis) when present at a uniform concentration (Zigmond and Hirsch 1973; Zigmond et al. 1981). We incorporated LTB4 directly into an agarose gel, at an optimal concentration for stimulation of cell movement (1 nM). Neutrophils (in medium containing the same concentration of LTB4) were added to a well in the gel and allowed to migrate from their starting position for 75 min, videotaped, and analyzed over the next 15-min period.

To assess the initial orientation of the cells within the field of view, we determined the average angle of cell migration during the first 2 min of the 15-min observation period. As shown in Fig. 1 a, the distribution of cell orientations was not random, but rather a majority of videotaped cells pointed away from the starting well, or forward (27/43 or 63%), in spite of the absence of a gradient. We postulated that this initial forward orientation might be the result of the following: (a) a mass action effect, in which most cells enter the field of observation from the direction of the starting well coupled with (b) a directional persistence, the well-described tendency of neutrophils to continue in their recent direction, rarely making sharp turns, even in the absence of an orienting gradient (Allan and Wilkinson 1978; Zigmond et al. 1981; Shields and Hatson 1985; Burton et al. 1987). Together, these two phenomena could result in an initial forward bias in cell orientation. The forward bias could also result from real chemotaxis because of, for example, cellular chemoattractant destruction. To distinguish between these possibilities, we analyzed cell behavior in more detail.

Consistent with previous reports, cells migrating in a uniform field in our assay displayed considerable directional persistence: after 5 min, only ∼50% of cells had turned >90° from their starting direction; by 10 min, ∼90% of cells had turned (Fig. 1 b). To the degree that cells persist in their initial directions, an initial forward bias in cell orientation would be expected to lead to a forward bias in subsequent cell movements. Therefore, any analysis of the cells' chemotactic behavior must be corrected for the cells' initial orientation. To assess the relationship between the initial and subsequent direction of cell movements, we calculated each cell's FMI during an initial 2-min observation period, and then during subsequent 4, 7, or 9.5-min observation periods. The FMI is the ratio of the net distance the cell progressed in the forward direction (away from the starting well) to the total distance the cell traveled as it wandered through the videotaped field (see Materials and Methods). The FMI measures the efficiency of a cell's forward migration during a given time interval. (In experiments involving migration to a distant attractant source, the forward migration index is equivalent to the McCutcheon index used by previous investigators to compare a cell's most direct path to a gradient source to its total path length [McCutcheon 1946; Zigmond 1974]).

Fig. 1c,Fig. e, show the relationship between the initial and subsequent forward migration indices for cells migrating in a uniform field of LTB4. Fig. 1 c shows the subsequent FMI, calculated over a 4-min period (from t = 2–6 min), plotted against the initial index, calculated over the first 2-min period of observation (t = 0–2 min). The regression line through these data has a positive slope (+0.55), indicating that cells that start out moving forward tend to continue to progress forward during the subsequent 4-min period, and that cells that initially move backward tend to continue to progress backward. The correlation between the initial and subsequent FMI is statistically significant (correlation coefficient r = 0.58, significantly different from zero with P < 0.0001). The slope of the linear regression line of the initial versus subsequent FMI is an indicator of the degree to which cells persist in their initial direction; therefore, it can be considered a persistence index. Fig. 1d and Fig. e, plots the cells' forward migration indices, as calculated over longer intervals (of 7- and 9.5-min duration), against their initial migration indices determined in the first 2 min of observation. Again, there is a significant positive correlation between the cells' initial and subsequent movements, but it becomes less prominent as cells are tracked for longer intervals (when the subsequent migration index is determined over 7 min, from t = 2–9 min, the slope = 0.33, and r = 0.41, P = 0.01; when determined over a 9.5-min period, from t = 2–11.5 min, the slope = 0.32, and r = 0.42). The correlation between the initial and subsequent forward migration indices, and the reduction of this correlation over time, is consistent with the effect of directional persistence on cell movement, and with the decay of this effect as cells are followed for a longer time interval. Cells displayed similar behavior when migrating in a uniform field containing both IL-8 and LTB4 (not shown).

The plots comparing the cells' initial and subsequent movement (Fig. 1) describe, in essence, neutrophils' turning behavior. To understand the data presented in Fig. 1 e more fully, it is useful to consider several example cells. Cell A pointed forward during the initial 2-min analysis period, as indicated by its positive initial FMI (+0.92), and it continued to progress forward during the subsequent 9.5-min analysis period. Cell B, like A, pointed forward during the initial 2-min analysis period, but did not maintain this course throughout the subsequent period. Cell B has a negative FMI during the 9.5-min tracking period (−0.46), indicating that, overall, this cell went backward during that time, and therefore, must have turned from its initial direction. Cell C pointed backward during the initial period, but turned during the subsequent period because, overall, it progressed forward during the subsequent 9.5-min analysis period (subsequent FMI = +0.26), whereas cell D also initially pointed backward, as evidenced by its negative initial forward migration index (−0.79), and it continued to progress backward during the subsequent period.

Once the relationship between the cells' starting directions and their subsequent progress is known, it can be used to assess whether cells display a directional turning bias. To do so, we asked to what degree an average cell progresses forward if it starts out pointing neither forward nor backward, that is, pointing to the left or right, with an initial FMI of zero. This value is equivalent to the y-intercept of the regression line that fits the initial versus subsequent migration index data. Note that the y-intercept of the plot for cells migrating in a uniform field of chemoattractant, in our system, is always close to zero (Fig. 1, c–e), so that a cell starting out with no bias forward or backward, on average, makes no net progress forward or backward. In fact, this is the behavior expected for cells migrating in the absence of a chemotactic gradient. Thus, by comparing cells' initial direction to their subsequent movement, we have arrived at a measurement of directed cell movement normalized for cells' starting orientations. This measurement, the y-intercept, describes the turning bias associated with true chemotaxis and is hereafter termed the chemotactic bias. The regression line through the cells' initial versus subsequent forward migration indices is therefore: subsequent FMI = chemotactic bias + (persistence index) × (initial FMI).

Measurement of Chemotactic Bias in a Gradient

Next, we assessed the behavior of cells migrating in an LTB4 gradient. In this experiment, we videotaped cells migrating from their starting well towards an LTB4-containing well 2 mm away. The initial and subsequent migration indices were determined for each cell as above. In this and the following experiments, the subsequent FMI was determined over a 9.5-min period only. The plot comparing initial and subsequent movement reveals several noteworthy features of the cells' behavior (Fig. 2). First, like cells migrating in a uniform field, cells that start out migrating forward tend to progress forward during the subsequent analysis period (i.e., then tend to have positive subsequent forward migration indices). However, in contrast to cells migrating in a uniform field, even cells that start out pointing backward, or towards the cell starting well, tend to make net progress forward (up the LTB4 gradient) during the subsequent 9.5 min. Consequently, the y-intercept of the regression line through these data, or the chemotactic bias, is positive (0.34, with a 95% confidence interval of 0.24–0.43). This indicates that, in this setting, an average cell that started out pointing orthogonal to the LTB4 gradient makes net progress forward, toward the LTB4 source, during the subsequent 9.5 min. Based on these data, we can conclude that cells exposed to this LTB4 gradient display a significant turning bias, and tend to progress towards the gradient source, independent of initial orientation and of directional persistence. The minimal residual effect of directional persistence is apparent in the slight positive slope of the regression line through these data.

The experiments that follow were analyzed in the same way, and in each case the y-intercept of the regression line relating the initial to subsequent forward migration indices is presented as the population's chemotactic bias.

Neutrophils Migrating from a Local Chemoattractant Source Can Orient and Chemotax Towards a Distant Chemoattractant

We were initially interested in understanding the ability of neutrophils to migrate away from one chemoattractant source in response to another, more distant agonist (Foxman et al. 1997). To this end, we tracked neutrophils migrating from a well containing IL-8 towards a well containing LTB4 (and vice versa). For the chemoattractant sources, we selected a concentration of LTB4 and IL-8 each eliciting optimal neutrophil migration from a distant well. At these concentrations, cells migrate the same distance toward a distant IL-8 source in the presence and absence of a local LTB4 source (and vice versa) (Foxman et al. 1997). This behavior is observed in spite of the fact that quantitative measurements of chemoattractant concentrations indicate that the mean concentration and gradient slope of the local agonist should itself be able to mediate chemotaxis of neutrophils in the opposite direction (Foxman et al. 1997).

We reasoned that this migration in the context of opposing or even conflicting gradients could represent either true chemotaxis or, possibly, enhanced chemokinesis in the region of chemoattractant overlap. To distinguish between these possibilities, we assessed cell migration behavior in the presence of opposing gradients as follows: cells were placed in a well with either IL-8 or LTB4 (the local well), and the other chemoattractant was added to the distant well 2 mm away. From 75 to 90 min, cells 400–700 μm from the local agonist (1,300–1,600 μm from the distant well) were filmed and their migration bias assessed, exactly as described above for the previous experiment. We found that the mean velocity of migrating cells was the same (24–27 μm/min) whether one or both chemoattractants were present, suggesting that altered motility could not explain cell behavior. Furthermore, neutrophils displayed a true chemotaxis toward a distant IL-8 source in the presence of a local LTB4 source, equivalent to that displayed in the absence of LTB4; similarly, cells chemotaxed towards a distant LTB4 source equivalently, in the presence or absence of a local IL-8 source (Fig. 3 a).

Additional control experiments were performed to determine whether IL-8 and LTB4 sources, at the concentrations used here, create functional gradients at a nearby site (400–700 μm). In these control experiments, we allowed naive neutrophils to migrate toward an IL-8 or LTB4 source from a starting well positioned closer to the chemoattractant, so that these cells would be 400–700 μm away from the source during the relevant time interval (75–90 min). Under these conditions, neutrophils exhibited significant chemotaxis to both a local IL-8 source and a local LTB4 source (Fig. 3 b), confirming that in the previous experiment (Fig. 3 a), a functional orienting gradient of the local agonist exists. We conclude that, under these experimental conditions, neutrophils display true chemotaxis to a distant agonist source even when migrating down an effective gradient of a local agonist.

Neutrophils Can Integrate Directional Signals from Chemoattractant Sources Presented at an Angle

The above results suggest that neutrophils can integrate competing directional signals in such a way that they continue to migrate directionally. One possibility is that this integration, in effect, involves a vector sum of the competing orienting signals. If so, we reasoned that it should be possible to demonstrate such vectorial integration by asking neutrophils to respond to two equidistant agonist sources presented at an angle. We adjusted the under agarose assay by cutting three wells in a triangular configuration, and introducing neutrophils into one well and chemoattractants into the other two wells.

We performed a series of assays in which neutrophils and chemoattractants were added to the wells and cells were allowed to migrate for 2.5 h. Photographs of cell migration patterns are shown in Fig. 4. When only one well contained chemoattractant, neutrophils migrated towards that well only. When both wells contained the same chemoattractant, either LTB4 or IL-8, neutrophils migrated furthest in the direction of each chemoattractant well. This is the migration pattern that would be expected if each cell migrated up the steepest local chemotactic gradient encountered. (From previous gradient measurements [Foxman et al. 1997], we know that there is a trough of lower agonist concentration up the midline, and that the steepest local gradients cells would experience would, thus, be in the direction of each chemoattractant source well.)

However, notably when one well contained IL-8 and the other contained LTB4, cells migrated furthest in a direction between the two chemoattractant sources. This is the pattern that would be expected if the direction of the entire migrating population is influenced by both chemoattractants. The possibility that this pattern could result from a combined effect of the chemoattractants on cell velocity is unlikely, since cells migrate with about the same velocity in the presence of either chemoattractant as they do the presence of both together (on average 20–30 μm/min; see for example Fig. 3 a). This result confirms that most neutrophils can respond to both agonists, and suggests that the cells' integrated response to two different agonists can result in predominant cell targeting to a point between the sources, which is consistent with a model of vectorial integration of the two signals.

A Role for Cellular Memory: Neutrophils' Chemotactic Behavior Depends upon Their Previous Chemoattractant Environment

We have shown that neutrophils behave as if they can integrate directional information from competing chemotactic signals (Fig. 4). On the other hand, in the initial experiments presented in Fig. 3, we saw that neutrophils can chemotax away from a local chemoattractant source towards a distant source of a different chemoattractant, without a noticeable effect of the local agonist on the cells' chemotactic bias. Why do neutrophils appear to ignore the local gradient in this context? One possibility is that cells somehow prioritize signals from a distant gradient over those from an otherwise equivalent local agonist source: this might be possible, for example, if the strength of the perceived orienting signals (and thus the behavior of cells) is influenced by the cells' prior history.

To evaluate this possibility, we altered the conditions of the assay: we placed the IL-8 and LTB4 wells closer together (1.5 mm), and used slightly different amounts of IL-8 and LTB4 (0.5 and 0.1 pmol, respectively), so that cells coming from either direction would enter the common central region (600–900 μm from each well) at 75 min. If the cells' chemotactic bias simply reflects an integration of the gradient vectors present, we anticipated that the cells in the middle region would behave the same way regardless of the direction from which they had migrated.

We observed that, under these conditions, cells that had migrated into the central region between the two wells always displayed true chemotaxis towards whichever agonist was presented in the distant well: neutrophils that had migrated from LTB4 into the central region displayed a significant chemotactic bias toward the initially distant IL-8 source, and vice versa. Furthermore, the chemotactic bias cells displayed towards an initially distant chemoattractant source was only slightly lower in the presence of an opposing gradient of the other chemoattractant (Fig. 5 a, top, and Table).

One possibility is that cells originating in one chemoattractant source well alter that chemoattractant source in such a way that it no longer generates a functional gradient (i.e., by degrading the chemoattractant). To address this issue, the same experiment was performed with neutrophils starting simultaneously in both chemoattractant source wells. To distinguish cells that originated in the LTB4-containing well from cells that originated in the IL-8–containing well, cells were labeled with either a green or an orange nuclear dye and cell movements were tracked using confocal microscopy. The chemotactic biases of these distinct cells populations were evaluated as cells migrated within a central region between the two chemoattractant sources (cells 550–950 μm from each well at 75 min). Again, we found that cells that had migrated into the central region between the two wells always displayed true chemotaxis towards whichever agonist was presented in the distant well (Fig. 5 a, bottom, and Table). By the end of the observation period, cells had intermingled significantly (Fig. 5 b). Because both cell populations were present in the central region simultaneously, we conclude that cells from different starting locations display different chemotactic behavior even when experiencing an identical chemotactic field. These results suggest that cellular chemotaxis in the presence of conflicting gradients is influenced by cellular memory, which alters the perceived strength of the orienting signals in a given chemotactic field based on the cells' previous chemoattractant environment, in such a way that cells respond preferentially to gradients of novel chemoattractants.

Previously, we have shown that leukocytes can navigate in a step-by-step fashion through complex arrays of chemoattractants (Foxman et al. 1997). Here, we have sought to understand the mechanisms whereby leukocytes can respond sequentially to regulatory cell–derived chemoattractants. Our experiments reveal two fundamental properties of leukocyte chemotactic responses: an ability of neutrophils to integrate competing directional signals, as if responding to the vector sum of orienting gradients present, and the dependence of the resulting directional responses on the cells' prior history (i.e., on cellular memory of the recent chemoattractant environment). We propose that these interrelated properties allow leukocytes to navigate successfully through complex chemoattractant arrays. We shall discuss the results of our experiments in the context of these concepts.

Vector Integration: Leukocytes Can Integrate Orienting Signals from Distinct Chemoattractant Sources

The present studies were initiated to understand the cellular responses that allow neutrophils to migrate efficiently from one agonist source to another (Foxman et al. 1997). Our analysis revealed that neutrophils display true chemotaxis to a distant chemoattractant source, even when the local agonist source also produces an effective (orienting) reverse gradient at the same position and time.

Migrating neutrophils are polarized, displaying a discrete leading edge and trailing uropod, even in the absence of an orienting gradient (Allan and Wilkinson 1978; Zigmond et al. 1981; Shields and Hatson 1985). Gradients of leukocyte chemoattractants are thought to elicit chemotaxis by determining the direction of a cell's leading edge and, hence, the direction of cell locomotion, independent of cell velocity, adhesivity, or other factors. If cells can integrate orienting signals, then in the presence of overlapping gradients, the direction of cell chemotaxis would be expected to reflect the vector sum (the average magnitude and direction of) the orienting signals present. If the chemoattractant gradients are not aligned, as when cells are migrating away from one source towards another, the orienting signals would compete. In this situation, whichever gradient produces a stronger orienting signal would be expected to determine the cells' direction of orientation and, thus, determine the direction of cells' chemotactic response. The apparent ability of cells to ignore a local chemoattractant source, thus, may reflect an integrated response to the opposing orienting signals, but one in which the cells' chemotactic bias is determined preferentially by the (presumably more compelling) distant gradient.

In fact, we were able to demonstrate convincingly that neutrophils can integrate conflicting directional signals by showing that, in the presence of two balanced agonist sources 60° apart, neutrophils migrate furthest in a direction between the agonist sources. This observation not only confirms that most neutrophils can respond efficiently to both of the two agonists used in this study (IL-8 and LTB4), but that at the population level, the direction of cells' displacement is determined by the vector sum of the orienting signals provided by each gradient. In the extreme case, this implies that two opposing gradients (of exactly matched orienting strength) should be able to cancel each other out. In fact, under carefully selected conditions, we have been able to create such a balanced competition in which neutrophils between IL-8 and LTB4 sources behaved as if in the presence of a uniform chemoattractant field (Foxman 1999).

One prediction from these observations is that in the presence of a stable array of regulatory chemoattractants, cells will eventually find a central region in which opposing orienting signals are perceived as equivalent. Within this region, cells would migrate back and forth until they encountered additional signals. Such signals could include a newly introduced chemoattractant source, or other classes of guidance cues such as counterreceptors for leukocyte adhesion molecules (Loike et al. 1995). Even if other influences were irrelevant, the phenomenon of directional persistence (and of cellular memory see below) would ensure that in this setting cells would be broadly distributed throughout a region of regulatory cell activation, as in a tissue site of inflammation.

Cellular Memory: Leukocytes Prioritize Distant or Novel Chemoattractant Sources

In studying neutrophils' chemotactic behavior in the presence of opposing chemoattractant gradients, we discovered that cells presented with identical chemotactic fields exhibit different chemotactic behavior depending upon their history. Neutrophils arriving in a central region between IL-8 and LTB4 sources displayed opposite chemotactic responses, depending on the direction from which they came, even when neutrophils were added simultaneously to both wells and assessed as they migrated towards each other in the same central location.

If cells' directional bias is determined by the integration of opposing orienting signals, how can neutrophils display different chemotactic biases when migrating in exactly the same gradient conditions? We propose that these experiments define a phenomenon of cellular memory of the recent chemoattractant environment, in which cells' responsiveness to agonists in the recent chemoattractant environment is selectively diminished, ensuring that they can preferentially respond to newly arising chemoattractant sources within tissues.

What is the basis of this change in cellular responsiveness? Neutrophils and other leukocytes are known to adjust their sensitivity to a chemoattractant upon exposure to that chemoattractant. At the chemoattractant concentrations that efficiently elicit chemotaxis (0.01–10× the receptor Kd), the conditions most relevant to our experiments, cells undergo a process known as adaptation. Adaptation is a feature of many sensory systems using G protein–coupled receptors, including animal visual systems (Koshland 1980), and is thought to reflect adjustment in agonist-specific signaling mediated by homologous receptor desensitization (or resensitization and receptor recycling). An adapting system adjusts its sensitivity according to the background level of stimulation it receives. Early studies showed that neutrophils experiencing slight increases in chemoattractant level exhibit a transient response, and then adjust to the new chemoattractant concentration (Zigmond and Sullivan 1979). This adaptation takes many seconds to several minutes, after which neutrophils can again respond to an additional step increase in chemoattractant concentration. However, the dose of chemoattractant required to elicit a neutrophil response increases as cells adapt to higher ambient chemoattractant concentrations (Seligmann et al. 1982).

Cells that have adjusted to a certain chemoattractant concentration can regain their prior sensitivity to lower agonist levels when the chemoattractant is removed, but this process takes time. Neutrophils that experience a sudden decrease in chemoattractant concentration show morphological changes, which subside within 2–6 min, depending on the magnitude of the decrease (Zigmond and Sullivan 1979). The full recovery of chemotactic responsiveness can take even longer. For example, in studies by Goldman and Goetzl 1984, neutrophils preincubated with low, chemotactic levels of LTB4 (0.3, 1, or 3 nM), washed, and allowed to recover, showed a significant reduction in chemotactic responsiveness to LTB4 even when assayed starting 10 min later. Cells preincubated with slightly higher concentrations (10–30 nM) exhibited no chemotaxis to LTB4 even when assayed after a 10-min recovery period (Goldman and Goetzl 1984). Cells that have experienced extreme, saturating chemoattractant levels (∼100× the receptor dissociation constant, or Kd) undergo not only receptor desensitization, but also extensive receptor internalization, and can require a long recovery time (20–60 min) to regain former receptor levels (Sullivan and Zigmond 1980; Zigmond et al. 1982; Samanta et al. 1990; Chuntharapai and Kim 1995) and signal transduction efficiency (Wilde et al. 1989). We hypothesize that the time delay required to readjust cellular sensitivity after a reduction in ligand exposure is the basis for the phenomena we observe, in essence altering cells' perception of the relative strength of local chemotactic signals as a function of the cells' recent chemoattractant environment. A model is shown in Fig. 5 c.

Temporal Versus Spatial Components of Gradient Perception

How can this model explain the apparent ability of cells to migrate between two chemoattractant sources presented at an angle? Leukocytes and other cell types, such as Dictyostelium amebas, are thought to sense chemotactic gradients using a spatial mechanism, in which they instantaneously calculate the differential occupancy of chemoattractant receptors across the cell body (Zigmond 1974; Zigmond 1977; Devreotes and Zigmond 1988). Although distinct chemoattractant receptors could theoretically each initiate a unique signaling cascade leading to chemotaxis, current studies suggest that signals through different chemoattractant receptors converge on a common chemotaxis-initiating pathway (Arai et al. 1997; Neptune et al. 1999). If different chemoattractant receptors share common intracellular signaling pathways, at any given instant a cell is not likely to be able to tell whether the signals it is receiving are from one agonist or another. Thus, at any instant, neutrophils presented with competing gradients should respond as if in the presence of a single agonist.

However, the cells' ability to adjust their sensitivity to different attractants independently can allow vector integration of distinct orienting signals over time. In our model, when two sources of the same chemoattractant are present, a cell simply migrates up the steepest local gradient it encounters. When two different chemoattractants are present, a cell also migrates up the steepest local gradient. However, as it migrates closer to one chemoattractant source and becomes less sensitive to that agonist and, thus, relatively more responsive to the other, it will turn and move towards the second agonist, potentially even across the midline. Thus, the perception and response to complex chemoattractant arrays involves both instantaneous spatial gradient perception at each moment, and changing perceptions of individual gradient vectors over time. In this model, it is the temporal component that allows cells to integrate orienting vectors from different chemoattractant sources.

The Significance of Cellular Memory for Leukocyte Homing In Vivo

The phenomenon of cellular memory has fundamental implications for directing leukocyte homing in vivo, namely, whenever a cell experiences competing attractant gradients, memory will promote cell migration towards a novel chemoattractant. This model is outlined in Fig. 6.

In this study, we show that neutrophil chemotaxis in overlapping chemoattractant gradients is guided by vector integration of orienting signals, and by cells' memory of their prior chemoattractant environment. (In vivo, of course, these mechanisms would operate in conjunction with regulated adhesion and cell activation to control cell direction.) Our observations support a model in which a leukocyte's chemotactic bias in competing chemotactic gradients is dynamically regulated by its previous experiences. This type of regulation can promote leukocytes' sequential migration to (and navigation through) the gradient sources present within a tissue, allowing combinations of chemoattractants to effectively guide cells to their unique destinations.

We thank Bill Brown for enthusiastic and helpful assistance with statistical analyses and Sally Zigmond, Eddie Bowman, and Dan Campbell for critical review of this manuscript.

This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) and an award from the Department of Veterans Affairs (to E.C. Butcher). Ellen F. Foxman was supported by the Medical Scientist Training Program (grant number GM07365) from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences. Eric J. Kunkel was supported by the NIH Immunology Training Grant (grant number 5T32-AI07290).

Allan
R.B.
,
Wilkinson
P.C.
A visual analysis of chemotactic and chemokinetic locomotion of human neutrophil leukocytes
Exp. Cell Res.
111
1978
191
203
[PubMed]
Arai
H.
,
Tsou
C.L.
,
Charo
I.F.
Chemotaxis in a lymphocyte cell line transfected with C-C chemokine receptor 2Bevidence that directed migration is mediated by beta-gamma dimers released by activation of G-alpha-i-coupled receptors
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA.
94
1997
14495
14499
[PubMed]
Baggiolini
M.
,
Dewald
B.
,
Moser
B.
Human chemokinesan update
Annu. Rev. Immunol.
15
1997
675
705
[PubMed]
Becker
S.
,
Quay
J.
,
Koren
H.S.
,
Haskill
J.S.
Constitutive and stimulated MCP-1, Gro-alpha, beta, and gamma expression in human airway epithelium and bronchoalveolar macrophages
Am. J. Physiol.
266
1994
278
286
Burton
J.L.
,
Bank
H.
,
Law
P.
Videoanalysis of chemokinesischaracterization of speed, persistence, and orientation in an agarose assay
Annu. Clin. Lab. Sci.
17
1987
389
397
Campbell
J.J.
,
Foxman
E.F.
,
Butcher
E.C.
Chemoattractant receptor crosstalk as a regulatory mechanism in leukocyte adhesion and migration
Eur. J. Immunol.
27
1997
2571
2578
[PubMed]
Chuntharapai
A.
,
Kim
K.J.
Regulation of the expression of IL-8 receptor A/B by IL-8possible functions of each receptor
J. Immunol.
155
1995
2587
2594
[PubMed]
Cook
D.N.
,
Beck
M.A.
,
Coffman
T.M.
,
Kirby
S.L.
,
Sheridan
J.F.
,
Pragnell
I.B.
,
Smithies
O.
Requirement of MIP-1α for an inflammatory response to viral infection
Science.
269
1995
1583
1585
[PubMed]
Devreotes
P.N.
,
Zigmond
S.H.
Chemotaxis in eukaryotic cellsa focus on leukocytes and Dictyostelium
Annu. Rev. Cell. Biol.
4
1988
649
686
[PubMed]
Dobos
G.J.
,
Norgauer
J.
,
Eberle
M.
,
Schollmeyer
P.J.
,
Traynor-Kaplan
A.E.
C5a reduces formyl peptide-induced actin polymerization and phosphatidyl(3,4,5)trisphospate formation, but not phosphatidylinositol(4,5)bisphosphate hydrolysis and superoxide production, in human neutrophils
J. Immunol.
149
1992
609
615
[PubMed]
Förster
R.
,
Mattis
A.E.
,
Kremmer
E.
,
Wolf
E.
,
Brem
G.
,
Lipp
M.
A putative chemokine receptor, BLR-1, directs B cell migration to defined lymphoid organs and specific anatomic compartments of the spleen
Cell.
87
1996
1037
1047
[PubMed]
Foxman
E.F.
,
Campbell
J.J.
,
Butcher
E.C.
Multistep navigation and the combinatorial control of leukocyte chemotaxis
J. Cell Biol.
139
1997
1349
1360
[PubMed]
Foxman, E.F. 1999. The combinatorial control of leukocyte chemotaxis. Ph.D. Thesis. Program in Immunology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA. 1–161.
Gao
J.
,
Wynn
T.A.
,
Chang
Y.
,
Lee
E.J.
,
Broxmeyer
H.E.
,
Cooper
S.
,
Tiffany
H.L.
,
Westphal
J.K.
,
Murphy
P.M.
Impaired host defense, hematopoiesis, granulomatous inflammation, and type 1-type 2 cytokine balance in mice lacking CC chemokine receptor 1
J. Exp. Med.
185
1997
1959
1968
[PubMed]
Gillitzer
R.
,
Ritter
U.
,
Spandau
U.
,
Goebeler
M.
,
Brocker
E.
Differential expression of gro-alpha and IL-8 mRNA in psoriasisa model for neutrophil migration and accumulation in vivo
J. Invest. Dermatol.
107
1996
778
782
[PubMed]
Glabinski
A.R.
,
Tani
M.
,
Strieter
R.M.
,
Tuohy
V.K.
,
Ransohoff
R.M.
Synchronous synthesis of alpha- and beta-chemokines by cells of diverse lineage in the central nervous system model of mice with relapses of chronic experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis
Am. J. Pathol.
150
1996
617
630
[PubMed]
Goldman
D.W.
,
Goetzl
E.J.
Heterogeneity of human polymorphonuclear leukocyte receptors for leukotriene B4
J. Exp. Med.
159
1984
1027
1041
[PubMed]
Gonzalo
J.
,
Lloyd
C.M.
,
Kremer
L.
,
Finger
E.
,
Martinez
C.
,
Siegelman
M.H.
,
Cybulsky
M.
,
Guiterrez-Ramos
J.
Eosinophil recruitment to the lung in a murine model of allergic inflammationthe role of T cells, chemokines, and adhesion receptors
J. Clin. Invest.
98
1996
2332
2345
[PubMed]
Hopken
U.E.
,
Lu
B.
,
Gerard
N.
,
Gerard
C.
Impaired inflammatory responses in the reverse Arthus reaction through genetic deletion of the C5a receptor
J. Exp. Med.
185
1997
749
756
[PubMed]
Kitayama
J.
,
Carr
M.W.
,
Roth
S.J.
,
Buccola
J.
,
Springer
T.A.
Contrasting responses to multiple chemotactic stimuli in transendothelial migration
J. Immunol.
158
1997
2340
2349
[PubMed]
Koshland, D. 1980. Adaptation. In Bacterial Chemotaxis as a Model Behavioral System. Raven Press, NY. 107–125.
Kurihara
T.
,
Warr
G.
,
Loy
J.
,
Bravo
R.
Defects in macrophage recruitment and host defense in mice lacking the CCR2 chemokine receptor
J. Exp. Med.
186
1997
1757
1762
[PubMed]
Loike
J.D.
,
el Khoury
J.
,
Cao
L.
,
Richards
C.P.
,
Rascoff
H.
,
Mandeville
J.T.
,
Maxfield
F.R.
,
Silverstein
S.C.
Fibrin regulates neutrophil migration in response to interleukin 8, leukotriene B4, tumor necrosis factor, and formyl-methionyl-leucyl-phenylalanine
J. Exp. Med.
181
1995
1763
1772
[PubMed]
Luster
A.D.
Chemokines—chemotactic cytokines that mediate inflammation
N. Engl. J. Med.
338
1998
436
444
[PubMed]
McCutcheon
M.
Chemotaxis in leukocytes
Physiol. Rev.
26
1946
319
336
Nelson
R.D.
,
Quie
P.Q.
,
Simmons
R.L.
Chemotaxis under agarosea new and simple method for measuring migration of human polymorphonuclear leukocytes and monocytes
J. Immunol.
115
1975
1650
1656
[PubMed]
Neptune
E.R.
,
Iiri
T.
,
Bourne
H.R.
G-alpha-i is not required for chemotaxis mediated by G-i-coupled receptors
J. Biol. Chem.
274
1999
2824
2828
[PubMed]
Richardson
R.M.
,
Ali
H.
,
Tomhave
E.D.
,
Haribabu
B.
,
Snyderman
R.
Cross-desensitization of chemoattractant receptors occurs at multiple levels
J. Biol. Chem.
270
1995
27829
27833
[PubMed]
Sabroe
I.
,
Williams
T.J.
,
Hebert
C.
,
Collins
P.D.
Chemoattractant cross-desensitization of the human neutrophil IL-8 receptor involves receptor internalization and differential receptor subtype expression
J. Immunol.
158
1997
1361
1369
[PubMed]
Samanta
A.K.
,
Oppenheim
J.J.
,
Matsushima
K.
Interleukin-8 (monocyte-derived neutrophil chemotactic factor) dynamically regulates its own receptor expression on human neutrophils
J. Biol. Chem.
265
1990
183
189
[PubMed]
Schroder
J.
Cytokine networks in the skin
J. Invest. Dermatol.
105
1995
20s
24s
[PubMed]
Seligmann
B.E.
,
Fletcher
M.P.
,
Gallin
J.I.
Adaptation of human neutrophil responses to the chemoattractant N-formylmethionylleucylphenylalanine
J. Biol. Chem.
257
1982
6280
6286
[PubMed]
Shields
J.M.
,
Hatson
W.S.
Behavior of neutrophil leukocytes in uniform concentrations of chemotactic factorscontraction waves, cell polarity, and persistence
J. Cell Sci.
74
1985
75
93
[PubMed]
Spanaus
K.
,
Nadal
D.
,
Pfister
H.
,
Seebach
J.
,
Widmer
U.
,
Frei
K.
,
Gloor
S.
,
Fontana
A.
C-X-C and C-C chemokines are expressed in the cerebrospinal fluid in bacterial meningitis and mediate chemotactic activity on peripheral blood-derived polymorphonuclear and mononuclear cells in vitro
J. Immunol.
158
1997
1956
1964
[PubMed]
Sullivan
S.J.
,
Zigmond
S.H.
Chemotactic peptide receptor modulation in polymorphonuclear leukocytes
J. Cell Biol.
85
1980
703
711
[PubMed]
Tomhave
E.D.
,
Richardson
R.M.
,
Didsbury
J.R.
,
Menard
L.
,
Snyderman
R.
,
Ali
H.
Cross-desensitization of receptors for peptide chemoattractants
J. Immunol.
153
1994
3267
3275
[PubMed]
Wenzel
U.O.
,
Abboud
H.E.
Chemokines and renal disease
Am. J. Kidney Dis.
26
1995
982
994
[PubMed]
Wilde
M.W.
,
Carlson
K.E.
,
Manning
D.R.
,
Zigmond
S.H.
Chemoattractant-stimulated GTPase activity is decreased on membranes from polymorphonuclear leukocytes incubated in chemoattractant
J. Biol. Chem.
264
1989
190
196
[PubMed]
Zigmond
S.H.
Mechanisms of sensing chemical gradients by polymorphonuclear leukocytes
Nature.
249
1974
450
452
[PubMed]
Zigmond
S.H.
Ability of polymorphonuclear leukocytes to orient in gradients of chemotactic factors
J. Cell Biol.
75
1977
606
616
[PubMed]
Zigmond
S.H.
,
Hirsch
J.G.
Leukocyte locomotion and chemotaxisnew methods for evaluation, and demonstration of a cell-derived chemotactic factor
J. Exp. Med.
137
1973
387
410
[PubMed]
Zigmond
S.H.
,
Sullivan
S.J.
Sensory adaptation of leukocytes to chemotactic peptides
J. Cell Biol.
82
1979
517
527
[PubMed]
Zigmond
S.H.
,
Levitsky
H.I.
,
Kreel
B.J.
Cell polarityan examination of its behavioral expression and its consequences for polymorphonuclear leukocyte chemotaxis
J. Cell Biol.
89
1981
585
592
[PubMed]
Zigmond
S.H.
,
Sullivan
S.J.
,
Lauffenberger
D.A.
Kinetic analysis of chemotactic peptide receptor modulation
J. Cell Biol.
92
1982
34
42
[PubMed]
Zlotnik
A.
,
Morales
J.
,
Hedrick
J.A.
Recent advances in chemokines and chemokine receptors
Crit. Rev. Immunol.
19
1999
1
47
[PubMed]

1.used in this paper: FMI, forward migration index; IL-8, interleukin 8; LTB4, leukotriene B4; r, correlation coefficient

Portions of this work are included, along with additional experiments, in Dr. Foxman's Ph.D. dissertation (Foxman 1999).